Select Committee on Education and Employment Minutes of Evidence


Supplementary memorandum from Dr Michael Nelson

DIET, IRON STATUS, AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION

  Recent evidence from the US and the UK shows that cognitive function is poor in children with poor dietary status but no overt signs of deficiency, and that intervention to improve diet (eg supplementation studies, breakfast interventions) results in improved cognitive function and learning ability. Iron deficiency is the best nutritional documented cause of poor cognitive function in UK school children. Nutrients which enhance iron absorption (eg vitamin C) are also important. Improved nutrient intakes related to breakfast consumption are associated with better cognitive function and scholastic achievements.

RECOMMENDATIONS:

1.   Guidelines for school meals should be specified with respect to both foods and nutrients

  Enusuring good cognitive function requires feeding guidelines which promote healthy eating patterns and adequate nutrient intakes.

2.   Iron status should be monitored in adolescent girls

  Girls who are in the bottom quarter of the distribution of iron status have an increased risk of impaired cognitive function. School catering should pay particular attention to the promotion of good iron status. Girls with poor iron status may require iron supplements to boost cognitive function and scholastic performance.

3.   Schemes for the distribution in schools of free or subsidised fruit and vegetables are likely to have widespread benefits

  Higher intakes of vitamin C are associated with better iron absorption, better iron status and hence better cognitive function. Patterns of healthy eating would be established early. Risks of heart disease, some cancers, stroke and hypertension in adulthood would be reduced.

4.   Schools should have a "whole school" approach to nutrition

  Messages in the classroom should be reinforced in the canteen and tuck shop through appropriate catering practices. Programmes to improve nutritional knowledge and behaviour must link activities which include pupils, teachers, and caterers.

  The studies outlined on the following pages provide evidence in support of these recommendations.

PRIMARY SCHOOL CHILDREN

Tiniakos F. Iron status and cognitive function in poor inner city primary school children. MSc thesis. University of London. 1993.

  Location:   Four primary schools located in a poor area of Central London.

  Sample:   129 children aged 6.5-9.5 years.

  Measurements:   Food frequency questionnaires.

    Finger prick blood sample.

    Two tests of cognitive function (digit span and coding).

  Findings:  Children with poor iron status had poor cognitive function.

    1.  Four children (3 per cent) had haemoglobin levels less than 12.0 g/dl indicating iron deficiency anaemia.

    2.  Nine children (7 per cent) had ferritin levels less than 20mg/l indicating poor iron stores.

    3.  There was a statistically significant correlation between digit span and total iron intake (r=0.197, P=0.0033) which was not explained by age.

    4.  Table 1 shows that there were trends of increasing cognitive function with higher haemoglobin status. The trends failed to reach statistical significance, however.

Table 1

Mean haemoglobin (Hb), iron intake, digit span and coding test scores in 129 children aged 6.5-9.5 years living in a poor area of Central London

Hb group

Hb (range)<11.611.6-13.0 >13.0P
Means
Hb11.212.3 13.80.000
Iron intake6.84.8 5.10.156
Digit span4.95.9 5.50.391
Coding10.310.6 11.30.285


Shovlin A. Iron status and cognitive and physical performance in 7-11 year old school children. MSc thesis. London University. 1993.

  Location:   One primary school in a poor area of Central London.

  Sample:   77 children aged 7-11 years.

  Measurements:   7-day food diaries completed parents' or guardians' help.

    Venous blood samples.

    Two tests of cognitive function (digit span and coding).

  Findings:  Children with poor iron status had poor cognitive function.

    1.  There was a statistically significant correlation between digit span and haemoglobin levels (r=0.23, P=0.018), not explained by age.

    2.  There were no other significant associations observed between cognitive function and measures of iron status or diet.

  The results from these two studies suggest that in children from poor backgrounds better iron status is weakly but positively associated with better cognitive function (digit span). Levels of iron deficiency anaemia are low but a substantial number of children are likely to have poor iron stores. This may become problematic in adolescence when the limited reserves of iron (in conjunction with low dietary intakes of iron and vitamin C) are not sufficient to maintain adequate growth and development, and cognitive function is limited.

SECONDARY SCHOOL CHILDREN

  Studies published in the UK in the mid-1990s suggested that poor iron status and iron deficiency anaemia were common (10 per cent to 20 per cent) in apparently healthy adolescent girls[28][29] (The rates in boys were only about 3 per cent). This appeared to be a consequence of arriving at puberty with low iron stores, eating a diet that was low in iron and/or vitamin C (often in an attempt to lose weight or on becoming vegetarian), and starting menstruation with its associated iron losses.

  Recent intervention studies in adolescent girls in the Baltimore, Md, USA and in North London suggest that:

    (a)  poor iron status (with or without the presence of anaemia) limits cognitive function; and

    (b)  improvements in iron status amongst those with poor initial status leads to better cognitive function within 8-10 weeks.

Bruner AB, Joffe A, Duggan AK, Casella JF, Brandt J. Randomised study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in non-anaemic iron deficient adolescent girls. Lancet 1996; 348:992-996

  Location:   Four High Schools in Baltimore, Maryland, USA.

  Sample:   716 girls 13-18 years old screened for non-anaemic iron deficiency (normal haemoglobin, low ferritin). 98 enrolled in intervention trial.

  Measurements:   Haemoglobin (g/dl), ferritin (mg/l) in verrous blood.

    Four cognitive function tests.

  Findings:   Ferritin increased by 18.2 mg/l in the intervention group compared with 3.5 mg/l in the control group. After eight weeks of iron supplementation, Hopkins Verbal Learning test scores increased significantly more in the intervention group than in the control group.

  The authors conclude that improvements in iron status are associated with improved learning ability.

Ash R and Nelson M. Iron status and cognitive function in UK adolescent girls: and intervention study. (in preparation 1999).

  Location:   One single sex and two mixed comprehensive schools in North London.

  Sample:   537 girls 11.5-15.5 years old (school years 7-10) in screening study to determine iron status. 131 girls 11.5-15.5 years old in placebo controlled iron supplementation intervention trial.

  Measurements:   Finger prick blood sample for measuring Haemoglobin (Hb: g/dl), packed cell volume (PCV: %), mean corpuscular haemoglobin concentration (MCHC: g/dl) and zinc protoporphyrin(ZPP: mg/dl).

    Venous blood sample used to confirm iron status in girls in intervention trial.

    Food checklist.

    British Ability Scale tests for verbal and non-verbal intelligence (IQ).

  Intervention:   Supervised administration of 3-5 iron tablets or placebo per week for 10 weeks.

  Findings:  Screening study. The lowest level of iron status occurred at the peak of the growth spurt (age 12-13 years). Using multiple iron status indicators, 16 per cent were classified as iron deficient anaemic (IDA), a further 11 per cent as iron deficient (ID), and 73 per cent as iron replete (IR). Twelve per cent of White girls, 15 per cent of Afro-Caribbean and 29 per cent of Asian girls were classified IDA. Prevalence of IDA was 21 per cent in girls from families with no earner, and 31 per cent in girls who had been vegetarian for less than one year.

  Intervention trial,  131 girls were recruited into a placebo controlled iron supplementation intervention study. Fifteen IDA girls were age matched with 48 ID and 68 IR girls. Iron status was confirmed using venous blood samples.

  Mean IQ scores at baseline were statistically significantly lower for the IDA girls (102) compared with ID (109) and IR (111) girls. These differences were partly but not wholly explained by differences in adiposity, maternal and paternal age, and socio-economic group. A computer based test which measured the time taken to tap specified keys (measure of motor-neural function) showed that the IDA group was significantly slower than the ID and IR groups.

  Figure 1 shows the changes in IQ scores pre- and post-intervention. The IDA girls given an iron supplement improved the most; IDA girls given placebo did not improve. Increases in IQ in other groups were consistent with known levels of change on repeat testing.


These results suggest:

  1.  IDA girls who remained anaemic after having been given the placebo were less able to learn than those who were iron deficient or iron replete.

  2.  Girls who were given iron supplements or who were iron replete initially were best able to learn.

BREAKFAST INTERVENTION STUDIES

Pollitt E and Mathews R. Breakfast and cognition: an integrative summary. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 1998; 67 (suppl):804S-813S.

  Papers presented at an International Symposium on Breakfast and Performance in 1995 are summarised and integrated with data published since that time. "The pooled data suggest that omitting breakfast interferes with cognition and learning, an effect that is more pronounced in nutritionally at-risk children than in well-nourished children. At the very least, breakfast consumption improves school attendance and enhances the quality of the students' diets."

  The paper suggests that missing breakfast is associated with poorer performance on short-term memory, visual discrimination of competing stimuli, verbal fluency, tasks of arithmetic, and stimulus discrimination. Some of the improvement in performance is likely to be due to higher levels of blood glucose. There is evidence that eating breakfast is associated with higher intake of nutrients and better nutritional status.

  Participation in Breakfast Programmes was associated with improved performance in scholastic tests and better school attendance. It was not possible to tell from the design of the studies if the scholastic improvement was due to improved nutritional status or to better school attendance and longer exposure to the learning environment.

CONCLUSIONS

  Good nutrition in school is likely to be a correlate of good educational achievement. There is strong evidence for a link between iron status and cognitive function in adolescent girls, and limited evidence in primary school children. To minimise the risks of iron deficiency and iron deficiency anaemia at ages 11-12, it is important that children arrive at the adolescent growth spurt with good nutritional status, particularly with regard to iron.

  Lack of breakfast is associated with poorer cognitive function and scholastic performance. Participation in School Breakfast Programmes is associated with better school attendance and better scholastic performance. Whether the improved performance is due to improved nutritional status or longer exposure to the school environment is not clear.

Dr M Nelson

November 1999


28   Nelson M, White J, Rhodes C. Haemoglobin, ferritin and iron intakes in British children aged 12-14 years: a preliminary study. British Journal of Nutrition 1993; 70:147-155. Back

29   Nelson M, Bakaliou F, Tivedi A. Iron deficiency anaemia and physical performance in adolescent girls from different ethnic backgrounds. British Journal of Nutrition 1994; 72:427-433. Back


 
previous page contents

House of Commons home page Parliament home page House of Lords home page search page enquiries

© Parliamentary copyright 1999
Prepared 14 December 1999