Select Committee on Education and Skills Minutes of Evidence


Examination of Witness (Questions 220 - 239)

MONDAY 7 APRIL 2003

PROFESSOR CAROL TAYLOR FITZ-GIBBON

  Jeff Ennis

  220. A particular hobbyhorse of mine, Professor Fitz-Gibbon—and I think you are alluding to the fact that this is something we ought to focus on—I always believe that whatever the educational institution, you need to have a good social mix in that educational institution for it to be successful or for it to have the potential to be more successful. Would you agree with that?

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) I would want to see evidence, and I was very interested in reading the evidence presented by Sally Tomlinson who referred to an experiment done in Stockholm. I did not have time to go to the library unfortunately, and I want to get Torsten Husén's biography and read about that, because I do not know why I had not heard about it before. That was a randomised control trial, as I understood it, of having schools that were segregated, or selective schools, versus comprehensive schools. They came to the conclusion in Stockholm that the comprehensive model worked best. I would believe it on those grounds, and I would like to read about it.

  221. Going back to the chart, you seem to be advocating more vertical grouping in the secondary school setting. Obviously, it happens more in primary schools at the moment, where you get vertically grouped classes.
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) Yes.

  222. Do you think that that would make a difference in terms of improving achievement at secondary level?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) Well, I think we should find out. I think also that a very good reason for using cross-age tutoring is that children enjoy it. Students enjoy it. We do need joy in work: this is their childhood; this is not just a matter of meeting government targets. This is their childhood, and they should enjoy it. The fact that they like learning by teaching should encourage us to use it. Also, it makes them nice to each other and very helpful.

  223. I guess that same philosophy supports the need to integrate more special educational needs pupils into that setting.
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) I think inclusion is basically a good idea, but if we want to be experimental about it, then—for example, let us look at gifted students, at the other end. Is it justifiable to identify some students as gifted and give them a special programme? You might argue that it is cost-effective because they need to go fast and they need to be together. So let us draw a borderline; but within the error of measurement around that borderline, randomly, put some into the gifted programme and others not—this is totally ethical because it is all errors and measurement in the borderline—and then follow up the progress and see if actually some children hate being labelled "gifted". "No, I am not", they say; they do not want to be labelled "gifted". Others may relish it. We have to do thousands of experiments to find out what works. We cannot just argue the results. We need to find out with good evidence, and then have more experiments to design interventions that do work. But we need to measure a lot of outcomes, not just achievement. Childhood is not just about achievement. What do you remember when you leave school?

  Chairman

  224. One of the things that one remembers is actually the school not as an individual but as part of a social system in the school. Does your methodology not tend to pinpoint just the individual achievement rather than—one of the things we found when we looked at a range of schools, in a number of cities, was that it does matter that if you have a school which is a social system of kids coming from relatively low-income families, with a background of not just low achievement but something more complex—no tradition of educational achievement from their parents or the people they know who live in their street and in their community—there is a sense in which one never gets the lift in those educational aspirations and achievement. There is a social system. Are you not always looking at the individual, and you cannot actually evaluate the social system of the school?

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) Well, we need to look at a lot of aspects. We do evaluate social systems. We get measures of racism and of alienation—and those vary from school to school quite substantially.

  225. Levels of alienation are not the same as saying that this is a school in an old mining community or an old ship-building community, where none of their parents have ever gone to higher education, where no-one ever had any aspirations for education at all; there are no books in the homes, or very few, and no parents reading to a child at an early age. Is that not a social system that your analysis is missing?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) It is a question of what you can alter. As a teacher, the students come to you. You cannot go back into their homes and change their homes; it is what you do with them in the time they come to school. They should not be doing homework in primary schools. It does not matter; it does not make any difference; it does not improve their achievement. It is what we do in school, and that must be aimed at making them happy, kind, nice individuals, and good parents subsequently by learning social skills as well as cognitive skills.

  226. So the evidence we took when we did the Early Years inquiry about the importance of early years stimulation, reading to children and playing games, creative play—you think before they get to school that does not matter.
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) I think a lot of these early childhood interventions need replicating. The one that has been most strongly promoted . . .

  227. SureStart?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) No, that is a new one, but the one from the States that SureStart was probably modelled on. The name escapes me just now.

  228. HeadStart.
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) HeadStart was one, yes, but that did not produce such strong evaluations. There was the—

  Valerie Davey

  229. HighScope.

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) Yes. As someone said at one of our evidence-based conferences: "Why has that not been replicated?" The initial data that was so well promoted—and the students were followed up—were 64 students. That was hardly a large sample. But at every stage of schooling, the schooling should be as good and as much fun as possible, not just pre-school—or we will lose them later.

  Chairman: This is all very interesting, but we must move on. I will ask David to look at league tables.

  Mr Chaytor

  230. To return to YELLIS for a moment, your argument in favour of that is that it provides constancy because it is the same each year, unlike Key Stage 3; but why does anybody buy it if it is the same each year? Why do they not just photocopy it previously?

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) I suppose they could.

  231. Is it exactly the same test year after year after year?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) Yes, but it is a lot of work to mark tests and then they need to be nationally standardised, so you need the national standardisation. We have a fantastic team of very clever youngsters running computers. They do all the matching and they do the value-added calculations; so the teacher gets a huge amount of work for nothing, plus some free software, plus conferences, plus help on the telephone and so on. Uniquely in the world, schools are supporting 65 staff in Durham, the biggest research group in UK education[2].

  232. But you said earlier that YELLIS was primarily for predicting GCSE scores; and now you have referred to its value as a measure of value-added. Does it work equally well as both?

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) If it did not have good predictive validity, it would not be a good measure for value-added; so it has to predict well.

  233. How is it different from the methodology that the DfES uses for its value-added calculation, which just draws the difference between Key Stage 3 point scores and GCSE point scores? What is your criticism of the methodology that has been used in the value-added scores that were published last year for the first time?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) If we can look at figures 3a distribution 3b: that refers to GCSE predicting A-Level, but could just as well refer to Key Stage 3 predicting GCSE. What we see is that each subject has a different line necessary, and there is no way any single line could represent those adequately. So where the DfES has gone wrong is in treating an A as an A as an A, no matter whether it is in physics or communication studies. It just is not true that the same challenge is in both subjects.

  234. Your criticism is entirely based on the variability of the grading in each subject, because in your submission you make an issue of the contrast between physics and sociology for example.
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) They are graded so they give a similar distribution . I have no problem with that. But you must not then evaluate the school. If they put a lot of students in for an easier subject they should not go up the league tables as value-added.

  235. Is it not conceivable that the value-added achieved in sociology is greater than the value-added achieved in physics? Is that not an equally valid conclusion that you could draw?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) On what?

  236. As I understand it, your argument is that because the levels of difficulty of individual subjects vary—and you quote sociology whereby the cohort entering sociology, by and large, has a lower level of performance than that for taking physics—but could not the reason for broadly the same level of grade distribution be that there is more value-added generated by sociology teachers? Is that not an equally valid conclusion?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) No, I think it is most unlikely. You cannot compare the teaching of physics and sociology. We can only look at the data and ask how difficult it is to get a child through physics or through sociology. Most teachers would agree that one was harder than the other. We can then see the effect of calling them all the same. The maths enrolment has declined. It is not easy to get people through A-Level maths and so on. It is a matter of comparing like with like, and that is where the DfES is unfair to schools. When I did a re-analysis of the data of schools that were told that they were below average, they had put students in for the difficult subjects in the same way the others had. But the other schools that were told they were above average, when in fact they were not on our calculations, had put students in for a lot of easy subjects. That was having an impact. The capping may have reduced that effect a little bit, but it is still a very unfair system that does not compare like with like. The first principle of a statistical model is that it models the process that produces the data. The process that produces each of these regression lines is the examining in that particular syllabus; and that is what should be modelled statistically.

  237. If there were absolute parity or absolute equivalence of grading between the subjects, would the method of calculating the point scores be acceptable? Do you think it is a sound method in itself?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) I do not think what the DfES does is sound. The grading of the subjects is perfectly sound and I have no problem with the same distribution in sociology as in physics. It is just let's not kid ourselves that it would be good for the country to move everybody out of physics so they would do well in sociology.

  238. That is not the point I am trying to get at. If the way in which the grading of physics and sociology, for the sake of argument, were done that satisfied to you that it accurately reflected the levels of difficulty, do you think the DfES's method of measuring the progress from Key Stage 3 to Key Stage 4 is adequate, simply on averaging of the point scores?
  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) It would be enormously difficult to constrain them to be exactly the same level of difficulty. The Scottish Office asked me this in 1990, they said, "What would happen if all the subjects were equated and given a distribution of grades that reflected aptitude?" No-one would have got a fail in physics and nobody would have had an A in art. You should be able to use the full range in every subject. Everybody knows that they are different subjects and of course the distribution of aptitudes will vary from subject to subject. So some students in physics might be absolutely tongue-tied in sociology. However those who have A-level maths have higher verbal scores than those with A-level English, on average, so it is not a simple thing. Then the subject should be taught in a way that is appropriate for the group of students that take that subject, and it should be graded to give a reasonable distribution. The analysis must not penalise schools that put students in for tough subjects because the country needs people to do maths, sciences and foreign languages. Students who have A-level maths earn 10% more than students who have not taken A-level maths and are doing the same job.[3] We must not reward schools for moving people out of those subjects.

  239. On GCSEs you make the point about the five A-Cs and indicate that they are not being very helpful. What is the issue there? You do not think that accurately reflects the schools' achievement? How else could it be done? If it were five A-Gs at GCSE, would that be more accurate?

  (Professor Fitz-Gibbon) No, the average points score.


2   Note by witness: In universities. Back

3   Note by witness: Report from LSE. Back


 
previous page contents next page

House of Commons home page Parliament home page House of Lords home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2003
Prepared 15 October 2003