Select Committee on Education and Skills Memoranda


Written evidence submitted by Dr Morag Stuart

to the

Education and Skills Select Committee

in advance of the

Committee Meeting on Reading, November 15th 2004.




Reading: Draft Terms of Reference

Remit: To examine departmental policy and guidance on the teaching of reading to children in schools, focusing on foundation-level education through KS 3. To consider whether any changes are necessary to improve current guidance/ policy.









Author notes.

Dr Morag Stuart is currently Reader in Psychology in the School of Psychology and Human Development at the Institute of Education, University of London.

She has some 16 years experience of teaching 4- to 8-year-old children in Inner London schools.

Towards the end of her school teaching career, she gained a first class honours degree in Psychology after 4 years of part-time evening study at Birkbeck College, University of London.

She then trained as an educational psychologist at the Child Guidance Training Centre, located in the Tavistock Clinic, London.

In 1986, she was awarded her PhD from Birkbeck College, University of London, for a thesis entitled "Phonological awareness, letter-sound knowledge and learning to read".

She has now been involved in research into reading and its development for over 20 years.

The terms of reference for this meeting of the Education and Skills Select Committee invited consideration of six linked questions:

1.  Whether policy/ guidance has a sound base in research evidence.

looking at relative weight given to synthetic/ analytic phonics, whole word/ language, onset/ rhyme etc.

2.  Putting policy into practice - how effectively is guidance being translated into practice? What variation exists in practice?

3.  Which children benefit from current approaches? Are all equally well - served by current policy guidance on reading?

4.  Introduction of early literacy strategies - teaching children to read from a very early age.

5.  The success or otherwise of current policies compared to those being pursued in other countries - paying due attention to differences dictated by different languages.

6.  The relative value of pre-literacy experience - by comparison to those countries with later age start to education.

I have dealt with each of these questions in turn and in the order in which they appear in the Terms of Reference, except that questions 4 and 6 are dealt with in a single section.

1.  Whether policy / guidance has a sound base in research evidence.

1.1.  As no references to research studies are given in any of the published documentation for the NLS, this question is more difficult to answer than it need be: we simply are not told what research evidence was used in developing, for example, the detailed term-by-term teaching plans set out in the Framework for Teaching, and therefore cannot examine the soundness of the evidence used. There is a post hoc review of the kinds of evidence that might seem to underlie the NLS Framework for Teaching (Beard, undated), but this does not cover much in the way of evidence about reading and its development.

1.2.  It is the basis for the Framework for Teaching that I wish to consider here. Three major influences on the Framework for Teaching are immediately apparent: the historical context in which the NLS was introduced, Reading Recovery, and psychological research into the role of phonological awareness in reading.

1.3.  Historical context.   When the NLS was first mooted, the major source of influence on teacher training with regard to the teaching of reading over the previous twenty to thirty years was the 'psycholinguistic guessing game' or 'whole language' approach (Goodman, 1976, 1986; Smith, 1971, 78, 82, 88, 94, 2004). This approach views skilled reading as 'sampling' the printed text to confirm 'predictions' built up from 'context'. It views reading as a process that develops as naturally as walking and talking. Therefore, children do not need to be taught to read; reading will develop naturally as long as children are in a print-rich environment where adults share books with them. The first director of the NLS was concerned that the new approach to reading teaching embodied in the NLS needed to take along with it teachers who had been trained in a very different philosophy and pedagogy. There has never been any valid and reliable scientific evidence to support the Goodman/Smith view of skilled reading and reading development. However, this view remains to this day influential among many teachers, a cause for concern in the light of the OISE report's identification of motivation to continue with the teaching approach advocated in the NLS as challenge to its future success (see below).

1.4.  Reading Recovery.  During the 1970s and 80s, Reading Recovery was very much associated with the Goodman/Smith view of reading and reading teaching. The value of Reading Recovery lies in its emphasis on early identification and intervention for 6-year-old struggling readers. It has also recently adapted its pedagogy to include phonological awareness and phonics teaching. Many of the first Literacy Consultants appointed in LEAs to provide in-service training and consultancy in implementation of the NLS were Reading Recovery trained teachers: thus, reading recovery has influenced the NLS both indirectly (through association with the Goodman/Smith school of thought) and directly (through Literacy Consultants).

1.5.  These two influences are apparent in the Searchlights 'model' of reading (illustrated in Figure 1 below), which is the only model of reading that the NLS presents to teachers.



This 'model' of reading has no basis in research evidence. Its acceptance into the NLS as a means of teaching teachers to understand reading is therefore at first sight surprising.

1.6 Psychological Research. Adoption by the NLS of a model with no foundation in research evidence is especially surprising as, from over thirty years of psychological research into skilled reading and reading development, we now have tried and tested models of the processes involved in printed word recognition as well as models of the factors that influence comprehension of written texts. Printed word recognition processes and text comprehension processes are clearly confounded in the Searchlights 'model', where readers are assumed to get to the meaning of a text by a combination of sounding out or recognising the words that comprise the text, and using contextual information and their knowledge of grammatical structure to predict upcoming words.

1.7 However, when we consider the historical context within which the NLS was first introduced, adoption of the Searchlights 'model' is less surprising, because this 'model' represents an uneasy attempt to produce a compromise between the different approaches to the teaching of reading that follow from two diametrically opposed conceptions of the processes involved in skilled reading and reading development: the Goodman/Smith approach (and its initial acceptance by proponents of Reading Recovery), and the approach that results from psychological research, which is outlined briefly below.

1.8   The approach taken by psychologists views skilled reading as consisting of two essential dimensions: printed word recognition, and text comprehension (e.g. Gough & Hillinger, 1980; Hoover & Gough, 1990). This is illustrated in Figure 2 below.






1.9   According to this view, reading development therefore requires that children develop (which includes being taught) procedures for recognising printed words, and procedures for understanding the texts that their word recognition skills allow them to decipher. Teachers need to understand the procedures required for success in each dimension. Psychological research has provided valuable insights into the nature and development of procedures necessary for this success in each dimension.

1.10 Although printed word recognition processes and text comprehension processes are clearly confounded in the Searchlights 'model' and in the Goodman/Smith approach, from which it in part derives, they are not confounded in the Psychological approach, nor are they confounded in children's minds. Different factors predict successful development of printed word recognition processes from those that predict successful development of the ability to understand printed texts. Moreover, there are children who cannot understand the texts they read, despite appropriate printed word recognition skills (suggesting a specific problem with reading comprehension and/or language comprehension generally). There are also children who, despite relatively poor word recognition skills, can make remarkably good sense of texts (suggesting a specific problem with word recognition processes, combined with a strength in language comprehension). That is, a double dissociation has been observed between the two dimensions, suggesting that they are indeed separable aspects of reading.

1.11 Given the huge amount of psychological research into reading and its development over the past thirty years, it is also disappointing that only one psychological theoretical perspective is easily discernable in the original word level work at Key Stage 1 in the NLS Framework for Teaching: that concerned with the role of phonological awareness in facilitating successful reading development proposed by Bryant & Goswami (e.g. Goswami & Bryant, 1990). Many aspects of this particular account have been hotly contested and refuted by subsequent research. This has been reflected in some of the changes made to early KS1 word level work in later versions of the NLS (e.g. Progress in Phonics), where the emphasis on rhyme and onset-rime units is much reduced.

1.12 The questions that the Select Committee is interested in under this heading (i.e. the relative weight given under the NLS to synthetic/analytic phonics, to whole word/language and to onset-rime, etc) all relate to this essential conflict between different conceptions of reading that is inherent in the Searchlights 'model'. I will be happy to answer any detailed questions in these areas.

2.  Putting policy into practice

2.1  In this section, I have relied on the Final Report of the External Evaluation of England's National Literacy and Numeracy Strategies (OISE report, Earl et al, 2003)[1]. Whilst this report recognises that, for both strategies, "the gains to date have been impressive", it also suggests "there is still considerable ground to be covered if significant and lasting improvement is the goal".

2.2 Two of the barriers to significant and lasting improvement identified in the

OISE report are (1) teachers' limited understanding of reading and (2) the fact that many teachers are not yet convinced that the NLS is worthwhile in terms of improving pupil learning. They support the goal of the NLS, but not necessarily the means of getting there.

2.3 Teachers' understanding of reading.

To date, both Initial and In-Service Training has concentrated on training teachers to deliver the NLS. It has not presented any research-supported model of the processes involved in skilled reading, or of the ways in which children are thought to develop these processes. The OISE report quotes Willows (2002, p1) to emphasise the dangers of this approach to teacher-training:

Training teachers to implement instructional methods when they don't truly understand the underlying rationale is futile. Without understanding, teachers do not have the knowledge to adapt an instructional strategy to address various student needs…..Without understanding, teachers can become inflexible and dogmatic, unable to integrate new research-supported practices into existing approaches.

Yet data collected by the OISE team indicate that most teachers believe they do have the necessary skills and knowledge to teach children to read. That is, teachers do not know that their understanding is limited.

2.4 However, limitations on teacher understanding were perceived by the majority of Consultants, who felt that "teachers still needed detailed classroom guidance, deeper subject knowledge and greater pedagogical expertise" (OISE report, p93), and by Headteachers, who made comments like: "there's another layer missing and that's the understanding of how children learn"; " (teachers) are very good at bashing out the curriculum, but they need to work on looking at where the children are at, listening to their needs. They still have a lot to learn about how children learn" (OISE report, p94).

2.5 This is a dangerous state of affairs, as "If teacher learning does not become a routine feature of ongoing practice, the principles behind the Strategies may be diluted or distorted by well-intentioned people who are unaware of the gaps in their understanding" (OISE report, p.134).

2.6 The persistence of large numbers of teachers unconvinced that implementing the NLS has led or will in the future lead to improvements in pupil learning carries with it the danger that focus on improving literacy will erode. In my opinion, those aspects of the NLS Framework for Teaching that are still somewhat controversial (e.g. the emphasis on word level work and phonics) are likely to be most threatened.

2.7 In my opinion, this threat relates directly to the lack of teacher knowledge about reading and to the failure of the NLS to educate teachers about reading.

3.  Which children benefit from current approaches? Are all equally well-served by

current policy guidance on reading?

3.1   The initial 'good teaching' supplied under the NLS clearly doesn't 'work' for everybody - if it did there would be no need for Additional Literacy Support (ALS), Early Literacy Support (ELS) and Further Literacy Support (FLS), or for Wave 3 individual attention for approximately 5% of children. Once this Three Wave version of implementation of the NLS has been in place throughout their school career for children at the end of Key Stage 2, if there are then still schools that do not meet the criterion of 65% of pupils achieving Level 4 at the end of KS2, we might need to consider whether this fuller version of the NLS works for all children.

3.2 Matters that would then need to be considered include teacher training, the

appropriateness of the targets set for 11-year-olds, and the appropriateness of the KS2 assessment procedures.

I would be happy to answer questions relating to the above matters.

4.  Introduction of early literacy strategies - Teaching children to read from a very

early age and

6.  The relative value of pre-literacy experience - by comparison to those countries

with later age start to education.

4.1 In the UK, we already start teaching children to read at least a year earlier than is the norm in most other countries. Now that most children appear to enter the Reception class at the start of the school year during which they will reach their 5th birthday, some children in Year R are only just four years old. This is not too young for children to be learning about reading, but in my opinion it is too young for all children to be formally taught to read and expected to be successful.

4.2 This is not to say that activities known to facilitate successful reading development should not be included in the Year R curriculum, or indeed into the Foundation Stage curriculum that covers 3- to 5-year olds. These activities should be fun. They should be presented as play. They should include language development activities as well as more traditional pre-reading activities. Some children (e.g. those for whom English is not the language spoken at home, those from socially deprived backgrounds) particularly need language development activities.

6.1 I am happy to answer questions about the kinds of activities that can be particularly beneficial to pre-school children.

5.  The success or otherwise of current policies compared to those being pursued in

other countries - paying due attention to differences dictated by different languages.

5.1  In this section, I am relying on the recent report from the Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS, Mullis et al, 2003), which compared reading achievements of 4th grade (9- to 10-year-old) children in 35 countries in 2001. The English cohort in this study was therefore the cohort against whom the success of the NLS was to be judged in 2002. I am indebted to Dr Rhona Stainthorp for the detailed analyses of relevant PIRLS data cited below.

5.2   One of the major impulses for the introduction of the NLS was a desire to reduce the

perceived tail of underachievement in reading, that is, to increase the number of 11-year- old children who reached the standard of reading deemed appropriate for an 11 year old.

5.3   Dr Stainthorp's detailed analysis of the PIRLS data suggests that this desired outcome

has not been achieved. Figure 3 (following page) shows the extent to which children at different levels of reading ability in six economically advanced European countries differ from the standard deviation from the average computed from data for all 35 countries. Children in these European countries do relatively well at all levels of reading ability when compared to the global averages: the standard deviations are all in a positive direction. However, the direction of slope is different for English children. In England, we do much better by our good readers and much worse by our poor readers. To some extent, this can be explained by the fact that English orthography is the most opaque alphabetic orthography of all, and therefore might present relatively more difficulty particularly for the least able children. However, French is also a relatively opaque alphabetic orthography, yet the French do relatively better by their least able children.

Figure 3

5.4   Thus, data in Figure 3 to some extent might result from language differences.

In Figure 4, only data from three economically advanced English speaking countries are presented.

5.5   Here we see similar descending profiles for England and New Zealand, with English

children doing relatively better at all ability levels than New Zealand children - somewhat ironic, given the New Zealand origins of Reading Recovery (Clay, 1972), where this programme has been in use for 30 years. However, Canada does well by children at all ability levels - may be there is something to be learned from teaching methods used in Canada.

6.  Final thoughts.

6.1 If I have been highly critical here of certain aspects of the National Literacy Strategy, this is because I am worried by the possibility, raised also in the OISE report, that what I see as the beneficial effects of the NLS on the teaching of reading are likely to be lost unless the serious concerns raised in my criticisms are addressed.

6.2 These worries are exacerbated by recent decisions to hand over responsibility for assessing reading at the end of Key Stage 1 to schools and teachers. I do not consider that the SATs administered at the end of Key Stage 1 provide reliable and valid estimates of children's reading ability, but I do believe that some form of standardised assessment of reading is essential during Key Stage 1.

6.3 I also think there are better ways of assessing reading at the end of Key Stage 2 than the KS2 SATs provide.

6.4 I would welcome discussion of the policy implications of the issues I have raised.

6.5 I am attaching as an appendix a copy of part of my previous critique of the NLS, presented to the Phonics Seminar, March 2003. The section reproduced in this appendix raises questions about the teaching of reading comprehension in the NLS.

References

Beard, R. (undated). National Literacy Strategy: review of Research and other Related Evidence. London: Department for Education and Skills, Standards and Effectiveness Unit.

Clay, M.M. (1972). Reading: The Patterning of Complex Behaviour. Auckland: Heinemann Educational.

Goodman, K.S. (1976). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. In H. Singer & R.B. Ruddell (Eds.). Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading. Newark, Del.: International Reading Association.

Goodman, K.S. (1986). What's the whole in whole language? Scholastic Press.

Goswami, U. & Bryant, P.E. (1990). Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gough, P.B. & Hillinger, M.L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 179-196.

Hoover, W.A. & Gough, P.B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127 - 160.

Earl. L., Watson, N., Levin, B., Leithwood, K., Fullan, M., Torrance, N. (2003). Watching and Learning 3: Final Report of the external Evaluation of England'. Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.

National Literacy Strategy Framework for Teaching (1998). London: Department for Education and Skills.

Mullis, I.V.S., Martin, M.O., Gonzalez, E.J. & Kennedy, A.M. (2003). PIRLS 2001 International Report: IEA's Study of Reading Literacy Achievement in Primary Schools. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.

Smith, F. (1971). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read. Mahwah, N.J.; London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Smith, F. (1978). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read 2nd edition. New York; London: Holt, Rhinehart & Winston.

Smith, F. (1982). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read 3rd edition. New York; London: Holt, Rhinehart & Winston.

Smith, F. (1988). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read 4th edition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Smith, F. (1994). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read 5th edition. Hillsdale, N.J.; Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Smith, F. (2004). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read 6th edition. Mahwah, N.J.; London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Willows, D. (2002). The Balanced Literacy Diet.

http://www.aasa.org/publications/sa/2002_01/willows.htm


1   commissioned by the Standards and Effectiveness Unit at the DfES, and implemented by a team from the Ontario Institute for Studies in Education (OISE).  Back


 
previous page contents next page

House of Commons home page Parliament home page House of Lords home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2004
Prepared 26 November 2004