Select Committee on Education and Skills Memoranda


Appendix Previous critique of reading comprehension teaching in the NLS Framework for Teaching.

Reading comprehension in the NLS.

I shall now turn to "reading comprehension" as set out in the NLS Framework for Teaching, and attempt to relate this to research evidence about the processes that are involved in reading (language) comprehension.

As I said earlier, we do not have two separate language comprehension systems, one for spoken and one for written language, but a single system that we have to learn to access from both oral and written input. The early and continuing emphasis in the NLS on identifying differences between 'book language' and oral language appears to assume that oral language development is pretty well complete, and that all children have developed oral language to an age appropriate level. Learning the nuances of difference between oral and written presentation is then all that is needed to facilitate comprehension of written text. But these are questionable assumptions. It is clearly not the case that oral language development is anywhere near complete during the early primary years. It is also certainly not the case for many children that oral language development is at an age appropriate level (Stuart, Dockrell & King, in press; Locke, Ginsborg & Peers, 2002). Therefore there should be much more emphasis, especially but not exclusively in Key Stage 1, on language development per se. Many children cannot produce a coherent oral narrative: do we know whether such children can comprehend oral narrative? Yet we expect them to comprehend written narrative.

Reading comprehension or literary criticism?

At Key Stage 2, much of the NLS Fiction text level work that is labelled "Reading Comprehension" might better be labelled "Literary Criticism". For example:

Year 4 term 2, children should be taught "to understand how the use of …

descriptive language can e.g. create moods, arouse expectations, build tension…."

Year 4 term 3, children should be taught "to describe how a poet

does or does not use rhyme…"

Year 5 Term 1, children should be taught "to analyse and compare poetic style."

Year 6 Term 2, children should be taught "to analyse the success of texts

and writers in evoking particular responses in the reader…"

These are ways in which children can demonstrate comprehension and meta-level understanding of use of language and of its formal properties; they are not ways to teach comprehension.

Failure to take proper account of research evidence.

More seriously, the NLS fails to make explicit use of research evidence accumulated over the past 30 years that has demonstrated some of the causes of failure to comprehend in children who have age appropriate word recognition skills (i.e. whose failure to comprehend cannot be due to word level failures which might be prevented by good early phonics teaching). Among the causes of comprehension failure so far identified are:

  • Poor knowledge of story structure
  • Poor domain (background) knowledge and/or failure to link this to incoming information
  • Problems with inference making
  • Problems with establishing causal and cohesive links
  • Problems in integrating information in text
  • Problems with working memory

Each of these is considered in turn below.

Poor knowledge of story structure

In the NLS work on comprehending fiction, there is lots of work on narrative (story) structure. However, to understand why this work is there and to teach it well, teachers need to understand the concept of story grammar. I think this work would also benefit from being removed from the "literary criticism" context in which it is embedded: children do not need to work on story openings and endings so that they can recognise or write good ones themselves, but so that they acquire the concept of the beginning and its scene setting function, and the end and its resolving function. This is immediately apparent from discussion of story grammar, in which a scene is set (the where and when), characters introduced and developed (the who), goals identified (the what), motives and intentions identified (the why), actions towards the goal are described (the how), problems encountered and how they are overcome are presented, and a resolution reached (the outcome). This work - on the when, where, who, what, why, and how - is scattered throughout both Key Stages but nowhere is it presented as a whole to the teacher with its purpose and importance clarified.

There is also explicit reference to chronology in narrative and to ways in which the passage of time can be represented. This is presumably to be explored across a range of the different genres explicitly identified (Sci-Fi, adventure, myths and legends, texts from different cultures, etc). Children are also to be taught the formal structures likely to be encountered in play scripts (stage directions, ways of indicating dialogue) and poetry (rhyme patterns, language use, etc). There is again no explanation of how knowledge of these kinds of structure affects comprehension -if indeed it does.

Poor domain (background) knowledge and/or failure to link this to incoming information

There is implicit acknowledgement of the importance of activating prior knowledge and linking this to incoming new knowledge, in the requirement in Key Stage 1 that children should relate story settings and incidents to their own experience. But domain knowledge is likely to be or to become more important in reading non-fiction. Successful teaching techniques for improving domain knowledge include showing films or videos to introduce new topics. Successful techniques for encouraging children to activate and use their prior knowledge within a domain include pre-reading discussion of the topic to be studied, so that children are made aware of what they already know in that area. This can be followed by discussion of what questions remain unanswered, so that children select texts and read with a question or questions in mind. These issues are explicitly mentioned with regard to non-fiction only in Year 2 Term 3, Year 4 Term 2. They should permeate the whole curriculum.

Problems with inference making

There is no reference at all in the NLS Framework for Teaching to the importance of inference in comprehension. This is alarming, given that research has shown that less skilled comprehenders have particular problems with making necessary inferences (Oakhill, 1984; Oakhill & Yuill, 1986; Oakhill, Yuill & Parkin, 1986). Moreover, it has also been shown that less skilled comprehenders' ability to make inferences can be improved by training that helps children to activate their prior knowledge and shows them how to draw on this to answer inferential questions (Hansen & Pearson, 1983; Yuill & Oakhill, 1988). Some kinds of inferences do depend on bringing prior knowledge to bear on the text. Jane Oakhill gives the following simple example:

"Jane was invited to Billy's birthday party.

She wondered if he would like a kite.

She shook her moneybox.

It made no sound!"

In order to make sense of this simple sequence, children must go beyond the literal text and fill in the gaps from their own prior knowledge. If you don't know that it is usual to buy a present for the birthday boy, you won't understand why Jane is wondering whether Billy would like a kite, you won't infer that she is planning to buy him a present and that this present should be selected please him. If you don't know that kites have to be bought and paid for, you won't understand why Jane is shaking her moneybox. If you don't know that money in a moneybox rattles when it is shaken, you won't understand the implications of the lack of sound from Jane's moneybox. If you were asked questions probing these gaps in the literal text, you would answer them either incorrectly or not at all.

It is important that teachers understand that inference from real world experience can be essential to understanding even the simplest shortest narrative text. Yet, the NLS Framework for Teaching makes no explicit mention of the development of inference skills.

Problems with establishing causal and cohesive links

Garnham, Oakhill & Johnson-Laird (1982) showed that less-skilled comprehenders with adequate word recognition abilities have difficulty in taking advantage of the cohesive links in texts that are created by use of anaphoric devices such as pronouns. For example, in the phrase "It was ready", it is impossible to know what it is that was ready. The pronoun 'it' in this phrase can only be interpreted with reference to information in another part of the text. In this case, 'it' has to be linked back to the sentence that preceded it, "Bill checked the cake in the oven". 'It' then clearly refers to 'the cake in the oven'. Our knowledge of gender and syntax allow us to know that 'it' doesn't refer back to 'Bill' - Bill is male and therefore would be referred to by the pronoun 'he'. Less skilled comprehenders' difficulty in interpreting cohesive devices such as anaphora means that they cannot establish even local links among the sentences in a text. This is not something that can be addressed solely by sentence level work, yet it is not mentioned at all in the NLS text level work.

Problems in integrating information in text

Less skilled comprehenders also have problems in integrating ideas presented in different parts of a text. This has been shown by Oakhill and her colleagues by asking children to detect anomalies in texts. The question asked is, "Does this story make sense?". The stories to be read are short passages that present inconsistent information in different parts of the text (e.g. moles cannot see very well; moles have very good eyesight). The less skilled comprehenders were much worse at recognising inconsistencies, suggesting that they do not automatically integrate incoming information (nor do they monitor their own comprehension). Again, there is nothing in the NLS framework that overtly addresses this issue.

Problems with working memory

The identification of working memory problems in some less skilled comprehenders is important in itself, since this is also likely to interfere with the ability to link ideas from different parts of the text (Yuill, Oakhill & Parkin, 1989), which is essential to obtaining an overview of the text, and therefore to main idea identification. But it is also important as an example of the kinds of cognitive processing difficulties that can exist within the minds of some children, and which prevent them from achieving an age-appropriate understanding of texts. Children who score poorly on tasks designed to assess verbal intelligence are also likely to have limits set on their ability to understand texts: if your listening comprehension is poor, then so will be your written comprehension. No account seems to have been taken of this unpalatable fact in setting attainment targets, or in designing a curriculum that is governed entirely by chronological age.

How could reading comprehension be better presented and taught?

At this point we might be wise to look westward, to the report produced by the National Reading Panel (NRP) in the US in 2000. This gives a brief overview of the path taken by research into reading comprehension since the late 1970s, as well as classifying and evaluating research studies. In the early days, studies investigated the effects of training children in the use of single strategies that were deemed likely to improve comprehension. Then combinations of strategies were trained. Finally, the implications for teacher training and teaching began to be examined.

To these ends, pupils (mostly from grades 3 through 6) have been trained to monitor their own comprehension, to make graphic aids to improve comprehension and memory, to make mental images, to acquire and use background (prior) knowledge, to generate and answer questions, to summarise, and to develop their knowledge of story structure. These activities have sometimes been conducted in shared learning situations in small groups, and have also sometimes been conducted across the curriculum. Effectiveness of training has usually been demonstrated on immediate post-tests designed to evaluate just what has been taught. There are fewer studies that have also been able to show improvements on standardised tests of reading comprehension. Most of the strategies trained are processes already shown to be lacking in less skilled comprehenders.

Implications for teacher education.

Despite the availability of this plethora of research, a recent observational study of ten classrooms (Pressley, Wharton-McDonald, Mistretta & Echeverria, 1996) found that teachers, whilst consistently identifying comprehension as one of their primary goals, were not directly teaching comprehension strategies to their pupils. Instead, they occasionally mentioned strategies, and presented pupils with test questions tapping understandings that would have developed from strategy use. This is likely to be due to the real difficulties encountered in teaching teachers to be successful in improving their pupils' comprehension. Duffy (1993) suggests that major changes in teacher education are required to help teachers become good strategy teachers, and emphasises the importance of taking account of the complexity involved in this kind of teaching, and the need for teachers to make creative adaptations to deal with that complexity. If we accept his views, then simply setting out what pupils 'should be taught' is extremely unlikely to lead to major improvements in children's reading comprehension.

Effective teaching methods.

Despite the difficulties inherent in equipping teachers to become successful in inculcating comprehension strategies, Pressley & Wharton-McDonald (1997) identify three methodologies for effective comprehension teaching: reciprocal teaching, direct explanation, and transactional strategies instruction. These are discussed in turn below.

Reciprocal teaching

Pupils are taught to use four comprehension strategies: predicting, questioning, seeking clarification, and summarising. Instruction takes place in small groups, with each pupil taking a turn to be the teacher. As teacher, they predict likely content from titles and related knowledge, pose questions about the reading and model summarisation. When there is confusion about ideas expressed in the text, they either predict upcoming content or seek clarification. The class teacher scaffolds these activities by giving prompts.

Direct explanation.

Teachers begin by explaining strategies to students and modelling these for them. Pupils then have practice in using the strategies in reading that is monitored by the teacher. The teacher provides additional explanation and modelling as needed.

Transactional strategies instruction.

This approach stems from the work of Pressley and colleagues in identifying successful implementations of comprehension teaching. The following characteristics of successful programs comprise the basis for transactional strategies instruction.

Several strategies are taught, including prediction based on the activation of prior knowledge; question generation; clarification seeking; mental imagery, and summarisation. Teaching is long term over several months. Strategies are directly explained and modelled. Teachers then coach as students practise. Students model strategy use for each other. Appropriate application of relevant strategies is taught. Teachers continued to model strategy use throughout the school day and across the curriculum.

The way forward.

It can be seen from the above that comprehension teaching in the US is more clearly based in research evidence both about the nature of comprehension difficulties and about effective intervention than is perhaps the case in the UK. It would seem that, to further improve reading standards in the UK, several things are needed. We need to collect the necessary data that will allow us to identify the real sources of the recent stall in progress: that is, data on standards of context-free printed word recognition, and data on performance on standardised tests of reading comprehension. We need to provide teachers with a model of reading that does not confound word recognition processes with those involved in comprehension. We need to ensure that all teachers are properly trained to teach phonics quickly and effectively in Key Stage 1. We need to provide teachers with research-based training in reading comprehension, so that they understand the likely causes of failure and know which kinds of strategies it is appropriate to teach to improve different aspects of children's comprehension. We need teachers who are both professional and expert in their understanding and their teaching. Only this can release the necessary creativity needed for teachers to be adaptable and well-informed in their teaching of reading.

Appendix References

Duffy, G.G. (1993). Rethinking strategy instruction: Four teachers' development and

their low achievers' understandings. Elementary School Journal, 93, 231-247.

Garnham, A.; Oakhill, J.V. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1982). Referential continuity and

the coherence of discourse. Cognition, 11, 29-46.

Hansen, J. & Pearson, P.D. (1983). The effects of inference training and practice on

young children's reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 391-417.

Locke, A.; Ginsborg, J. & Peers, I. (2002). Development and disadvantage:

implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Disorders of Communication, 37, 3-15.

Oakhill, J. (1984). Inferential and memory skills in children's comprehension of

stories. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 31-39.

Oakhill, J. & Yuill, N.M. (1986). Pronoun resolution in skilled and less-skilled

comprehenders: Effects of memory load and inferential complexity. Language and Speech, 29, 25-37.

Oakhill, J.V; Yuill, N.M. & Parkin, A.J. (1986). On the nature of the difference

between skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E.Morris and R.N. Sykes (Eds.). Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues, Vol 2. Chichester: Wiley.

Pressley, M.; Wharton-McDonald, R.; Mistretta, J. & Echeverria, M. (1996). The

nature of literacy instruction in ten grade 4/5 classrooms in upstate New York.

Pressley & Wharton-McDonald (1997). Skilled comprehension and its development

through instruction. School Psychology Review, 26, 448-466.

Stuart, M.; Dockrell, J. & King, D. (in press). Language intervention in preschool:

Talking time - Evidence for the development of oral language skills in classrooms. In K. Maridaki (Ed.). Children's Understanding of Mind: Empirical and Theoretical Approaches.

Yuill, N.M. & Oakhill, J.V. (1988). Effects of inference awareness training on poor

reading comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2, 33-45.

Yuill, N.M.; Oakhill, J.V. & Parkin, A.J. (1989). Working memory, comprehension

ability and the resolution of text anomaly. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 351 -361.





 
previous page contents next page

House of Commons home page Parliament home page House of Lords home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2004
Prepared 26 November 2004