Select Committee on Education and Skills Minutes of Evidence


Examination of Witnesses (Questions 20-39)

PROFESSOR BRYAN RODGERS, PROFESSOR JUDY DUNN, DR LEON FEINSTEIN AND DR AMANDA WADE

12 JULY 2006

  Q20  Mr Chaytor: So to some extent we are talking about historic factors rather than current factors?

  Professor Rodgers: Yes. If you look historically, for example, if you compare the different British National Birth Cohort Studies, in the 1946 Birth Cohort that I worked on, there were more children in that cohort whose parents died than whose parents divorced. By the time of the 1958 Birth Cohort Study there were many more divorces than parental deaths. That is in 12 years. So, at that time and in the immediate post-war period, you were seeing the effects of very, very rapid changes in divorce rates.

  Q21  Mr Chaytor: Was the immediate post-war period the period which showed the most rapid change in divorce rates?

  Professor Rodgers: Through until the early 1970s the divorce rate went up hugely. It multiplied over a period of 20 years.

  Q22  Mr Chaytor: So there have been two peaks, the immediate post-war period and the mid-seventies to mid-eighties, or two periods of rapid increase?

  Professor Rodgers: You have got to be careful about peaks. There is a post-war peak, which is probably due to what happened after the war, people coming back from the war and marriages not working out. There are also peaks with changes in legislation.

  Q23  Chairman: Making divorce easier.

  Professor Rodgers: Yes, and in all countries when no-fault divorce has been introduced you get this sharp peak, people who have probably been separated for some time and, finally, their divorce comes through. Aside of those peaks, you just see this very, very general trend across countries of increasing rates.

  Q24  Mr Chaytor: In terms of the other question, the relationship with social class, is there a relationship? Is that constant across English speaking countries?

  Professor Rodgers: It is not as strong as people imagine. If you broaden it out to take account of financial circumstances, some sense of, say, occupation as indicating social class, or education, people's educational qualifications are not terribly strong predictors of divorce. There is a tendency to have high divorce rates for those of lower education. It gets more complicated when you view it as couples. You find mis-matches where you have got one highly qualified partner and the other is not, so the nuances are quite interesting in that area, but the general trends are not as strong as a lot of people imagine. There is some evidence from one of the British Birth Cohorts, the 1958 Birth Cohort, which might explain why there is some confusion as to how strong that relationship is of financial circumstances, and it appears from longitudinal studies that families that divorce, their financial circumstances deteriorate before the divorce and it is quite marked. It is not a finding that has been reported on very often, but where it has been it has been very striking. It may be that if you measured the financial circumstances of families immediately before they separate, you might see a much stronger relationship than you do for much longer term predictors such as people's educational background. I have not seen anyone pursue that idea to see how that comes about. Is it because families hit hard times, unemployment, gambling problems, drug problems? All sorts of things might explain that, but it is not just a simple, stable relationship, there is a dynamic to that, which, as I mentioned, is this marked decline prior to a separation.

  Q25  Mr Chaytor: Moving on to the specific educational consequences of this, what are the major consequences in terms of educational achievements or performance of children in school as a result of separation or divorce? Accepting that there is no average, what is the range of impacts that have been identified?

  Professor Rodgers: I do not know if Leon or Judy want to dive in, but I will just say something and get it out quickly. It is partly in response to Rob Wilson's question to Amanda, and that is when you talked about is it better for families to stay together. There are two sets of quite interesting evidence, one of which relates to educational outcomes, that partly answer your question. There have been studies in the UK and also in the United States that have reported the educational progress of children in terms of measured test scores at different ages. They have looked at the kids who experienced parental separation through the course of that longitudinal follow up and what is interesting about those studies is that they are quite consistent in showing that the gap between the kids who experienced separation and those who did not, which I think Judy has made the point is not huge, it is not an enormous difference and there are huge diversities within those groups, but that gap that you see is actually present before the family separated, and it is about the same order of magnitude before the family split up as it is afterwards. When you ask the question, "What is the effect of separation?", you look at that and you think, "Is there any, because the gap was there before as it was afterwards?" I think partly with Rob Wilson's question one of the issues that arises is that, although the two groups the show this gap before and afterwards, individuals will move around, and I think that was part of Amanda's response to that, saying for some families that separate there may be a relief, for other families it could be quite disadvantageous for the kids to experience that separation. My best guess at an individual level is that some of the kids will show a deterioration in their school performance but other kids might actually show gains when their parents split up. Overall it does not look as though as a group there is a big change as a result of that, and there is a directly parallel example of what I think illustrates the same point when you look at very long-term outcomes, things I have studied myself in adults. In two of the British National Birth Cohorts we have looked at kids who actually spent their entire childhoods with two parents, two original parents who lived together all the way through their childhoods but then the parents divorced later, and those kids do just as badly in the long-term as the kids whose parents separated whilst they were children. That brings it back to that very original point I made that when you talk about the effects of separation on the kids you have got to be very careful about your terminology and what that implies. As I say, it may be that for individual kids this separation has a bad effect, on some it has a good effect, but overall they seem to be, on average, pretty much where they were before the families split up.

  Q26  Mr Chaytor: So why are we here this morning? I am just wondering whether we should all go home, if you are saying there is no relationship that can be identified.

  Professor Rodgers: The gap, as I say, is there before and that to me is the interesting question. Where does it come from?

  Q27  Mr Chaytor: The other question is what is the cause of the gap that is there before?

  Professor Rodgers: I think all the things that have been mentioned so far and that Leon particularly has talked about in terms family dynamics, in education, the parents' interest in the kids' educational progress and their relationship with the kids, and these things follow a long-term course rather than just a response to family separation. My background is partly in epidemiology as well as psychology, and when Leon talks about a risk factor I just see that as an opportunity to do something. A lot of my work now is built around policy implications of research and even if X does not cause directly an outcome, if you know that there is a group of kids there who have poorer outcomes on average than a group of kids over there, my next step is to say: what do you do about it? I do not really give a hoot what the direct cause of that difference is if you can do something about it. So, it is getting that broader understanding, I think Leon described it as a sign or an indicator, and it is visible. You know who these kids are, you do not have to go ferreting around to work it out, and also it is now 25% of them, and that is a big change, going back to a time when it might have been 5 or 10% of kids.

  Q28  Chairman: 5 or 10% of kids?

  Professor Rodgers: I am sorry?

  Q29  Chairman: The two comparisons you are making.

  Professor Rodgers: The risk factor now of parental separation exists in 25% of kids. Even if the disadvantage associated with that has not changed historically, the prevalence of the risk factor has changed enormously. If your interest is in policy and prevention, as a marker it becomes more useful.

  Q30  Chairman: Reading between the lines of what you and Dr Wade have said, basically, if a family is happy, whatever kind of family it is, if there is a good relationship and a supportive family unit there, that is the crucial thing whether it is within marriage or without marriage.

  Professor Rodgers: Yes. Quite a lot of them are not, and that is the point, what do you do about it?

  Q31  Chairman: Perhaps we ought to do more research on what makes happy families.

  Professor Rodgers: You also do stuff on families that do separate, and so we know there are groups at risk. We know that on average the kids do not do as well from this group. What do we do about it?

  Professor Dunn: You can look at the children who, in spite of the fact that they have gone through these risks, are going okay. That is one exercise we have just been doing in our study based in ALSPAC 1, and the three factors that stand out are a good, warm affectionate relationship with the mother, a good, warm affectionate relationship with the non-resident father and the mother's depression, the mother's mental state. Those three factors, if they are working in favour, as it were, and they are going the right way, 70% of the children who have been through this painful constellation of family changes are doing fine.

  Q32  Mr Wilson: Does that mean 30% are not doing fine?

  Professor Dunn: More than. I would have to show you the graphs and things, yes, but we were comparing children with a trajectory where the risk of behaviour problems was going up with children whose trajectory stayed low in terms of adjustment problems.

  Dr Wade: You were saying you are beginning to wonder why you are here, and looking at this.

  Q33  Chairman: David is!

  Dr Wade: What I wanted to say was that children's outcomes and educational attainments are one thing and clearly what they attain educationally is very important both for their own futures and, if you like, for the future of the nation, but that is about the future, and there is also the question of the present, and I think it is important to look at what can schools do for children who are living through difficult circumstances. School is somewhere where children spend a majority of their time and I think there is a great deal that schools can offer to children who are facing all sorts of adversities, but also it is quite important to listen to what children have to say about what matters and, again, what children want from their schools can differ. For some children who are living through adversity school is one place they can go to where they can feel normal, where they can forget about family problems, where they can focus on other things, and then there are another group of children who find it extremely difficult to cope with school because they are so overwhelmed by their problems, and I think that is where you can begin to think: what resources can we base in schools that might help children in those sorts of situations?

  Dr Feinstein: To highlight a point Bryan made earlier about transitions, there is this possibly two, three year period of vulnerability; so that even if the long-run effect on educational achievement may not be very strong, there is a very difficult short-term period which may lead in all kinds of directions for different outcomes to do with mental health, behaviour or other kinds of Every Child Matters outcomes. I do not think I would want to say that because the average effect on the population overall may be relatively small this is not an area that we need to think about. I think from an Every Child Matters point of view it is very important that the school does recognise these features of development of the children going through the school and recognise where there are issues of family difficulty. Family breakdown is one indicator of difficulties in the family environment, there are others, but the fact there are others does not mean that schools should be let off the hook in ignoring this one and thinking how do they provide support and a context for adjustment for children where family breakdown is occurring.

  Q34  Mr Chaytor: Others may want to come in on the question of what schools can do to support children and mitigate the effects, but just summing-up what you have said so far, would it be reasonably accurate to say that there are a number of factors that are directly related to poor educational performance or under achievement, which could be poverty, conflict, violence within the home, with educational achievement of the parents and lack of certain values within the family structure which may be exacerbated by separation but that separation would not be a key factor in under achievement? Is that an accurate way of looking at it or not? The starting point for this morning's session is the assumption that separation is an important factor. That is why we are having the seminar on the session of separation and not on poverty, or conflict, or parental abuse, or parental level of achievement.

  Dr Feinstein: I would say that in the population as a whole family breakdown was not the key explanatory factor for educational under achievement, but there will be a substantial minority of children, we may be talking about 0.5% of the population, for whom, nonetheless, it is a very salient feature because it occurs in combination with other significant risk factors, but it is not going to explain the great prevalence of educational low performance.

  Q35  Mr Wilson: I want to come back on something that Professor Rogers said. You said that some children have an educational gain from family breakdown in one of your previous answers. Can you quantify that? Is it a small number, is it a medium or is it a large number that have an educational gain?

  Professor Rodgers: You cannot quantify the number of kids who would have that, but it is an observation based on the fact that in a study, if you have a large group that shows—

  Q36  Mr Wilson: Is there a study? Have you evidence based on a study that confirms your opinion that some children have an educational gain from family breakdown?

  Professor Rodgers: I am saying that if the average stays the same and some have a loss, then there must be some that have a gain. You cannot have the average staying the same and some showing a loss. It is just not possible. There must be a counter-balance.

  Q37  Mr Wilson: I can see with the explanations that have been given so far that some children may well have a gain if they have got a violent parent, but I want to understand what sort of numbers, what sort of percentage we are talking about.

  Professor Rodgers: You would have to specify the degree of the shift. There are datasets that could do it. What you would have to do is simply come up with a number. Is it a fifth of a standard deviation, a third of a standard deviation of attainment? You would have to specify some criterion for what is considered to be a significant shift, up or down, and then, sure, you could quantify it.

  Q38  Mr Wilson: Does anybody else have any evidence in terms of numbers that might benefit from family breakdown?

  Professor Dunn: It is a very complicated question, because we have talked about separation as if that is the end of the story, but, of course, of that 25% of children whose parents separate, a lot of them spend a period of time with a single parent and a lot of them gain a step-parent, they may or may not be married but there is another adult in the family, and each of those steps carries risks, which probably include educational risks, so it is not a simple question.

  Q39  Mr Wilson: I do not expect any of the questions we ask this morning are going to be simple questions, judging by the answers we are getting so far.

  Dr Feinstein: I have seen studies in the US, because there was this change in policy towards trying to keep families together and researchers were interested in the fact that if the father may be violent then what is the damage going to be to the child of keeping the family unit together? I could not quote you the findings of those studies but I know people have looked at that and found substantial risks associated with keeping families together where there are good reasons, from the child's perspective possibly, for the family not to be held together, but the prevalence will depend, taking that example, on what kind of study you are looking at. If you do a study of families where there is a violent father present, then you may find very large negative effects of that violence on the child, that will be a very large effect, but it is a small prevalence. In the population as a whole, that might average out to not a huge gap, but the thing is substantial in itself.

  Chairman: Let us move on a little. Paul, you want to look at the research evidence, not that we have not been looking at the research evidence already.


 
previous page contents next page

House of Commons home page Parliament home page House of Lords home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2006
Prepared 7 November 2006