Joint Committee on the Draft Gambling Bill Minutes of Evidence


Examination of Witnesses (Questions 234 - 239)

THURSDAY 8 JANUARY 2004

PROFESSOR MARK GRIFFITHS, DR EMANUEL MORAN AND PROFESSOR JIM ORFORD

  Q234  Chairman: Good morning everyone. May I begin by welcoming Professor Griffiths, Dr Moran and Professor Orford to our proceedings; they will be answering questions in just a moment. May I also make the point that Elliot Grant from the Bill team is present and available to speak to us if spoken to, should we be straying from some meaning in relation to the Bill. May I ask you to note that this is a public evidence session and the transcript will be produced and placed on the internet. We think it unlikely there will be any division during this Committee, but, were that to happen, we would suspend for 10 minutes and clear the public gallery. May I also ask you to note that a full declaration of interests from members of the Committee was made at the beginning of the first meeting and information for witnesses and a note of those interests is available. Could I confirm to everyone that time is tight and they should not feel obliged to answer a question if they have nothing to add to what other witnesses have already said. This is particularly important where we have three witnesses now and then four witnesses later on. Do you think vulnerability to problem gambling is inherent in the very activity of gambling? Or are some people especially vulnerable as the licensing objectives appear to suggest?

  Dr Moran: The first point I would like to make is the notion of problem gambling is a rather dubious one. The Royal College of Psychiatrists, whom I am representing here, does not accept it as a valid concept. The condition really that is agreed by the WHO is pathological gambling; that is, all excessive gambling associated with disorder. I think we have to recognise that pathological gambling is a behavioural disorder and therefore it is heterogeneous: there are a variety of causes which are responsible for the disorder and they broadly fall into social and individual. Clearly, there are individual vulnerabilities. Among the social factors, I think the most important is the availability of facilities. Gambling does not occur in a vacuum; it is organised on a commercial basis, with all the trappings that go with that, and therefore there are commercial pressures. The availability of facilities and the social pressures encouraging gambling are very crucial to the disorder.

  Professor Griffiths: I would add that when it comes to problem gambling there are obviously vulnerable individuals but there are also some activities which are inherently more problematic and/or addictive than others. I am sure we will be getting into the area of, for instance, slot-machines later on, but we know for a fact that slot-machines are inherently more addictive than, say, a weekly lottery. We know, therefore, that the structural characteristics of how games are designed actually do induce problems in some people. Added with that are the vulnerabilities of individuals—we know there are vulnerable populations like minors, problem gamblers, the intoxicated, for instance. These are various segments of the population that do seem to be more susceptible to problem gambling.

  Q235  Chairman: To summarise, you are saying, Dr Moran, that this is a behavioural problem. We are having some difficulty ourselves in defining problem gambling. We went to GamCare yesterday and were given about four different definitions by four different people with a bad experience of gambling, but I think the behavioural element is what came through in what they had all said. Is there a vulnerability as well which arises from ignorance of how gambling works or a general lack of time-and-money management skills in individuals?

  Professor Griffiths: However you like to describe it, problem gambling is basically a syndrome. There are lots of overlapping consequences of the behaviour. How a person gets from being a non-gambler through to a social gambler and to a problem gambler will be different in different people. Some people will have a genetic susceptibility; for some people it will be the environment in which they were brought up; for some people it will be to do with the nature of the activity itself. You can look at a problem gambler and say they are doing x, y and z, and, even though how they got from a to b may be very different, what they display is very, very similar. So, yes, it is a behavioural outcome but there are psychological, social and biological antecedents to that particular behaviour.

  Professor Orford: I would agree with what has been said but I would say there is much clearer evidence that the potential for addictiveness of gambling is inherent in the activity itself. First year psychology undergraduates learning the laws of how people acquire habits which are difficult to break, are often shown a picture of a gambling machine because it is an almost perfect mechanism for creating a habit that is difficult to break. So, I would say, inherent in the activity, yes. Personal vulnerability there are many ideas, many theories about, but on the whole there is very little evidence for it. The main vulnerabilities lie in some more obvious things. Being male (versus being female) is one of the factors; being of low income is one of the vulnerability factors; and having a parent who gambled heavily is another factor. On some of the other personality ideas—whether it is to do with genetics, self-esteem, being an impulsive personality—there is very little evidence for that.

  Dr Moran: The notion that pathological gambling is either an illness or just plain ignorance I think ignores the fact, as Professor Orford has just indicated, that ultimately the activity of gambling inherently is habit forming. We see it illustrated in terms of the gaming machine which used to be known the "one-armed bandit". It speaks for itself. There are these elements to gambling which encourage participation and, going to extremes, ultimately leading to what has been referred to as "chasing losses". I think we have to recognise that at the end of the day we talk about "gamblers who lose" but ultimately all gamblers lose because the gambling industry has to make a profit. Where does it come from other than from the pool that goes into it via the punter? There is a variety of factors. I am quite prepared on behalf of the college to submit another paper to you which will spell these things out in a bit more detail, in terms of personal characteristics and broader issues.

  Q236  Chairman: We would be very happy to have that. Could I ask you to make one other observation. Some of the people we met yesterday who admitted to having a gambling problem slightly surprised us in the sense that their definition of a problem was not necessarily that they were losing a lot of money. In other words, they had not fallen into financial ruin: the problem was to do with the addiction in the sense that they had lost all enjoyment from gambling but they felt compelled to do it on a daily basis. Does that come within your scope of definition?

  Dr Moran: Certainly, Mr Chairman. Basically pathological gambling is excessive gambling which leads to social and psychological and financial difficulty. It can be any combination of these. Like all human behaviour, it is heterogeneous. Take crime, for instance: there is no one explanation for crime. Similarly, with pathological gambling there is no one explanation. In helping people with difficulties, one of the first things one has to do is to dissect out the elements of the disorder and one pathological gambler will be different from others. One can categorise them. I have made some attempt to do this in different varieties, from the individual factors at one extreme to social factors at the other end. But, as Professor Orford has indicated, the issue is very much in the same league as, say, crime, where there is a variety of factors and most of the factors are socially determined.

  Professor Griffiths: May I add one thing, Mr Chairman? We talk about addiction. Financial ruin is not the basis of any addiction that I know. Most of us on this panel who have studied addictions for many years would say that addictions are really about the activity being the single most important thing in that person's life and compromising everything in their life to do that particular activity, getting withdrawal symptoms if they cannot do that activity, having cravings for that activity if they are unable to do it, and using that activity as a reliable and consistent way of modifying their mood. Financial ruin does not come into that. You can find gamblers who fulfil all those criteria. Unfortunately, most gamblers do lose a lot of money along the way, but you will get gamblers who have maybe not lost money along the way but are still addicted.

  Q237  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: Do you accept the figures contained in the Gambling Prevalence Survey of the number of people who are affected either by a pathological disorder or problem gambling (depending on how you want to define it) of between 0.6 per cent and 0.8 per cent? If the figure is as low as that, it means that 99 per cent of the population does not have a problem.

  Professor Orford: May I, as one of the authors of the British Gambling Prevalence Survey, immediately come in. I think people have been rather naughty, I have to say, in interpreting the figures as saying that we have a low gambling—

  Q238  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: Which people?

  Professor Orford: The Government for a start.

  Q239  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: I thought that was what you meant but I felt the need to clarify it.

  Professor Orford: There are several points I would like to make about this. From a public health point of view, any disorder which was thought to affect one-third of a million people in the country would be considered to be a very serious public health issue. That is the first point. The second point is that the estimate is only an estimate and there is a confidence interval scientifically around that estimate. All you can say is that there are two other countries in the world that have done national surveys that appear to be statistically different from Britain, and one is higher and one is lower. Australia is significantly higher. That is clear. The other European country—and the only European country where a survey has been carried out—is Sweden, and that is just significantly lower. It is not possible statistically to say that the British rate that we found is significantly lower than the American rate or than the New Zealand rate, for example. That is the second point I want to make. The other point is that 0.6 or 0.8 is of course just the percentage of everybody in the country. That includes millions of people who do not gamble at all; millions of other people who only gamble on the National Lottery. As soon as you start to express it as a percentage of all people who gambled on anything last year or as soon as you start to express it as a percentage of young people between the ages of 16 and 35 or as soon as you start to express it as the percentage of people who bet on fruit machines last year, of course the percentage goes up.


 
previous page contents next page

House of Lords home page Parliament home page House of Commons home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2004
Prepared 7 April 2004