Joint Committee on the Draft Gambling Bill Minutes of Evidence


Examination of Witnesses (Questions 240 - 259)

THURSDAY 8 JANUARY 2004

PROFESSOR MARK GRIFFITHS, DR EMANUEL MORAN AND PROFESSOR JIM ORFORD

  Q240  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: You have anticipated my second question precisely.

  Professor Orford: My last point—and this is a really important point—is that nobody—and this is Mark Griffiths' area of expertise, I know—has mentioned the rate for adolescents. All the evidence from this country (the survey here) and the United States and elsewhere is that there is a negative correlation between age and problem or pathological gambling. The 16-24 year olds had the highest rate in our group, and if you take that down to the group we did not look at (because they were under 16s), adolescents, all the evidence is that the prevalence rate there is the highest. Surprisingly. I was surprised when I started to find out this. I do not know if Mark would agree, but probably the best evidence is Susan Fisher's evidence, of 10,000 pupils between the ages of 12 and 15 in 100 schools across England and Wales, finding a prevalence rate of over 5 per cent. So there are several factors. I do not think we should continue to say that Britain has a low rate of problem gambling. It does not.

  Professor Griffiths: On this 99 per cent figure that is always used, I do have to echo that the vast majority out there simply do not gamble or gamble on the activities that we know really are not problematic. We know that a bi-weekly National Lottery ticket does not cause immense problems for loads of people. It you looked at, for instance, slot-machine players who define themselves as slot-machine players, and looked at the prevalence of problem gambling in those individuals, it would be a lot higher than the 0.6 to 0.8 per cent you are mentioning. As you say, if you take age into account as well, if you look at males aged between 16 to 25 who play slot-machines, you will find that the proportion of problem gambling is very, very high. It is a significant public health concern, as Professor Orford has pointed out.

  Q241  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: You think there should be a new gambling Prevalence Survey, which was hinted at by Lord McIntosh when he gave evidence to us before Christmas, in order to bring the figures up to date. If such a survey is conducted, do you think it s necessary to define much better the problems one is looking at and to look at age and different activities?

  Professor Griffiths: I think prevalence surveys are a useful tool but prevalence studies do not tell us a lot. They certainly do not tell us anything theoretically about gambling. All they can provide is some benchmark data, which is useful for informing public policy on a particular issue. I reckon that if you did the prevalence survey again now, from the one that was done in 2000 you would find very minor differences. Okay, you might find a couple of per cent increase in terms of people playing slot-machines, or a few per cent down in terms of playing the National Lottery. It would be much better to do one post de-regulation, to see the real effects in terms of what de-regulation will do to this country in terms of problem and social gambling.

  Q242  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: Or look at the areas which are opposed to de-regulation and look at what the problems are there.

  Professor Griffiths: Yes. There are obviously some areas which we know, even since the year 2000—for instance, internet gambling, spread betting—have caught on dramatically. Those figures will be dramatically different. Obviously those particular areas, where we know there is a particular new form of gambling that has come in, would be interesting to look at. My guess is that if you did an identical prevalence survey tomorrow, using the same kind of things that we used in 2000, you would not find a massive difference in people's gambling behaviour at the moment.

  Professor Orford: I would have said the same until I reminded myself exactly when the first survey was done. It will be five years later this year. Most of the data was collected late in 1999. We always said and everybody associated with it said we ought to do this every five years

  Q243  Chairman: Should the Government pay for that?

  Professor Orford: Yes.

  Dr Moran: I agree with everything my colleagues have said but I would make one point. I would remind the Committee the incidence of schizophrenia is 1 per cent but nobody in this room would seriously suggest that it is not a very serious problem.

  Q244  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: Each of our distinguished witnesses has mentioned the word "habit" several times. I apologise if I am asking you to simplify a complicated question too much. What level of probability that a habit will be formed would you expect if, let us say, 10 people went for the first time into a place where there was gambling?—and I accept that machines are more addictive than others. What proportion of that ten people would be likely to form a habit? When the habit is formed, what level of difficulty, compared with other addictions, would you expect to break that habit?

  Professor Griffiths: I do not know about my colleagues but I think that is almost an impossible question to answer—and maybe that was a deliberately hard question. I would take you back to what habits are all about. I have always argued that almost any behaviour is potentially addictive because addictions really rely on constant rewards. If constant rewards are not there, you cannot possibly become addicted to something. It is one of the reasons why I have argued, for instance, on a bi-weekly lottery, that no one is going to become addicted to something that you play only twice a week. A slot-machine, where you can play 12 times a minute, is a very different kettle of fish. This is something where people can be rewarded financially, socially, psychologically every few seconds, and that is why people develop these habits. That is why habits are reinforcing and they lead to various addictive behaviours. Trying to say what percentage of individuals when they first go into an arcade will become "addicted", is an incredibly hard thing to do without knowing what the person's vulnerabilities are to start with, knowing more about the general population. I would be surprised if any one of my colleagues here would be able to answer that question.

  Q245  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: I am sorry about the level of difficulty. We are concerned obviously about machines, as has already been expressed. The idea now that gambling is just a leisure activity, another mainline leisure activity, gives us all concern where young people are concerned. We want to get some idea, with the new de-regulation in prospect, what increase in the gambling habit are we likely to see among young people.

  Dr Moran: To a certain extent, I agree: I think it is an impossible question really to answer honestly. It is a bit like: How long is a piece of string? I think we have to highlight the fact that in gambling you have a particular form of activity which functions on the basis of what psychologists refer to as operant conditioning; in other words, in terms of reward. The schedule of rewards is on the basis, again, of what psychologists refer to as intermittent variable ratio reinforcement; in other words, there is a reward which appears intermittently. Each time you do it, there is not a reward. It does occur, but it does not occur regularly. It is unpredictable. That schedule of intermittent variable ratio reinforcement is well-known, has been known for decades, to be the best system of habit formation. It is the basis upon which we all operate. If you phone your girlfriend every night, she gets bored with you. If you stop phoning her, she loses interest. The important thing is to phone intermittently and unpredictably. That is what gambling is all about.

  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: That is very helpful.

  Lord Mancroft: Very helpful.

  Chairman: In other words, you are saying you cannot win! That is the point.

  Q246  Lord Mancroft: Accepting what you have said, one of the areas on which we have been focusing and about which we are concerned is young people—and children in arcades, for example, is the obvious thing. With my interest in drugs and alcohol, we in our field—and I think you would agree with this—generally have the view that the younger children are when starting to use drugs or alcohol, the more likely they are to have problems later on. Is that true with gambling? I think I know the answer to that but I would like to hear it from you. The fact that AWP machines in arcades are not the most exciting things in the world and they are not really great reward givers, are they a factor, for those kids who play them, in those kids growing up to have gambling problems?

  Professor Griffiths: In answer to your first question, I know of at least four independent studies that have shown that the younger you start the more likely you are to have problems—even within the adolescent period. I did a study way back in 1990 showing that those kids who started playing slot-machines at the age of nine years of age or below were significantly more likely to have problems than those who started at the age of 12 or above, and I have seen another three surveys in this country alone which have actually replicated that particular finding. The interesting thing and, I suppose, the good news for adolescents is we know that with all, what I would call, risky but rewarding behaviours, the peak use, whether for drugs, alcohol or gambling, does peak in adolescence and young adulthood. You will find a lot of adolescents will mature out and spontaneously remit, and when they take on other things in their life, job, first baby, marriage, for instance, the problem gambling will often disappear. It is quite obvious that when you ask problem gamblers about their history of gambling they will have said they started in adolescence, but that does not mean, we know for a fact, that all people who have had problems in adolescence go on to be problem adult gamblers. We know for a fact that the prevalence rates of problem gambling in adolescence are at least twice as high as they are in adults. We know for a fact, even though this is a very vulnerable population, that the good news is a lot of it drops out. The gaming industry and the Government might turn round and say, "Well, if behaviour does drop out, why should we worry?" The reason we worry—and I am talking about kids—is because when you have, say, an 11 year old who for six years suffered educationally, got themselves a criminal record, et cetera because of their behaviour, that has affected them for the rest of their life, even though by 18 they may have got it completely out of their system. There is no doubt that some people do graduate. For instance, I followed 25 people through a three-year period. Only three people out of that 25 graduated on to other forms of gambling. They were all slot-machine players and the thing that they said got them off slot-machines is that the rewards, as you were pointing out, were not high enough. In the end the jackpot on an AWP machine is not something that intrinsically is going to keep people motivated or rewarded. When you introduce things like unlimited prizes, that is a good acquisitional factor to keep people gambling in the first place. AWP machines, although they are called "amusement with prizes" machines, are still gambling machines, and to young people, as far as I am concerned, they are still as addictive as any other slot-machine basically because of things like the operant conditioning process we have outlined, it becomes a repetitive habit pattern. At the end of the day, the jackpot prize, for instance, is most important in why people first start to play an activity. It is not necessarily the main motivating factor that actually keeps people developing and maintaining that activity.

  Dr Moran: Could I very much support that. From my written evidence, this is something I am particularly concerned about. It does seem to me that the period when in Britain we decided it was okay for children to play gaming machines was a time when it was not really understood some of the evidence that Professor Griffiths is talking about. We now do know that slot-machines are potentially addictive. We do know that they are particularly attractive for adolescents. The Budd Committee report stated very clearly, you will remember, their instincts were absolutely against allowing any kind of machine-playing for under 18 year olds. I have not looked at the NOP survey, but I have seen reports in the press that they asked 1,000 people around the country what their view was, and 82 per cent said they were against children being allowed to play machines of the low stake, low prize type. It does seem to me that the Government now has a responsibility to protect young people which 20 or 30 years ago it was not so clear. It is clear now. I would wish to argue very strongly that it is wrong for Britain to continue to do something which as far as we can make out no other country that has rules at all allows. I was told by a minister in DCMS that Finland allows playing at the age of 15, but that was the only country that has rules that the minister could actually find, apart from Britain, that allows this playing. I think it is quite wrong that they continue to be called "amusement with prizes".

  Q247  Viscount Falkland: On this question of the "amusement with prizes" machines, Lord McIntosh told us when he came to give evidence that those who wanted to abolish what had existed for many, many years would have to do a bit of research—the research has clearly been done, as you have shown to us—and show what harm they are doing before they will be convinced that this business should be cut out. We have very little time to convince Government—and I sense that my colleagues on this Committee are pretty well convinced that this is a dangerous area which needs to be looked at, and, if not curtailed completely, something has to be done. Would you just add a rider to what you have already said about this question. If it is allowed to continue as it is at present, without any break on it, would you expect our gambling rates to be higher instead of lower than the rest of the English-speaking world, for example?

  Dr Moran: Chairman, Hippocrates said "Life is short, the art long, experience is fallacious." The point really is that when you have the experience that we are being told about—namely that there are children playing fruit machines and yet the prevalence does not seem to have gone up—we have to see this in the social context. I agree with all that my colleagues have said, and I would remind the Committee that the availability of fruit machines to children over the last few decades has been in a setting of a public policy of unstimulated demand for gambling. That is a very crucial aspect of the whole situation. Certainly my experience and that of my colleagues is that a lot of the young people who get involved in fruit machines in the early/mid teens grow out of it. But then of course a lot of young people get into mischief and grow out of it anyway. I think one of the reasons they have grown out of it is because of the social climate. The Government is now proposing a free for all, abandonment of the policy of unstimulated demand, and I would certainly forecast that in that setting a lot of these young people who drop it will shoot into further disturbance in terms of pathological gambling.

  Q248  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: How important is it, do you think, that the children are gambling in arcades where their parents are playing higher value machines? What is the effect of watching perhaps their parents or their parents' friends in the reserved area where the high value machines are being played? What effect does that have?

  Professor Orford: It has been said that it is protective to have a parent there. But you can equally argue that it is quite the opposite—as seems to be the case. One of the vulnerability factors is the modelling, the demonstrating of gambling behaviour by a parent who also gambles. Being taken into an amusement arcade by a parent, seeing a parent playing a machine, seeing a parent win on a machine, seeing the excitement associated with winning is likely actually to model habitual gambling behaviour rather than protecting a person against gambling behaviour. I do not think having a parent present is necessarily a protective factor at all.

  Chairman: We are going to move on to what effect the parent will have on problem gambling and changes that are going to be made.

  Q249  Lord Walpole: The NERA Economic Consultants have reported that the Bill will result in up to one million—so this is not a percentage, it is a number—problem gamblers in the UK, representing a three to four-fold increase from the current level. Do you agree with this estimate?

  Professor Griffiths: I would say it is plausible. Again, always trying to put a figure on this is very hard to do. I think Professor Orford and I have both mentioned in our writings about something called the "availability hypothesis". Basically, where you increase opportunity and access to gambling, not only do you increase the number of regular gamblers but you will also increase the number of problem gamblers. I have gone on record as saying that may not be proportional. Tessa Jowell was quoted as saying that a 50 per cent increase in turnover of gambling would not lead to any increase in problem gambling whatsoever. I find the mathematics of that unbelievable. I certainly believe that where there is wide de-regulation and massive increased opportunity to gamble, you will get people gambling who have never gambled before. That is not to say they will necessarily become problem gamblers but you will see an increase in problem gambling as a result of massive de-regulation. Whether it is three or four times, however . . . But I do not know how they came to that particular figure, but it seems plausible.

  Professor Orford: I would agree. I do not think there is anybody who has looked at gambling who thinks other than the increased availability will lead to increased problems. The Australian productivity report, which is much the most thorough report, which has looked very carefully at that, came to that conclusion. I think one million is a figure plucked out of the air, to be honest. But suppose it was a 50 per cent increase, from a public health point of view—and my main argument is that I want the Government to see this from a public health point of view, which I do not think they are at the moment—a 50 per cent increase in any rate of disorder in society, putting it up to half a million, would be tremendously significant.

  Chairman: I want to bring in Jeff, because you have mentioned Australia.

  Q250  Jeff Ennis: Are there any conclusions we can draw from comparisons with other countries, in terms of what we need to learn in bringing in new legislation here?—particularly the Australian model.

  Professor Griffiths: I think there are always things to learn from other countries. However, the one caveat I would put on that is that every country has a different culture of gambling. You can look at prevalence surveys in other countries and ask yourselves why some countries seem to have higher prevalence rates than us. I actually think, even though abroad we are seen as a nation of gamblers, that we have a very strict regime in place which has actually minimised the amount of problem gambling amongst the adult population compared with other countries. In Australia recently they have had a massive casino expansion and they have suddenly seen a massive increase in the amount of problem gambling. They have put internet gambling out. They have had a moratorium on that because they have seen a massive increase in that. In Australia, if people want to go to gamble, they actually have to travel quite a long distance to do it; we live in a country where we have 60 million people all crammed into this little space and it is not very far for anybody to travel to gamble in this country. I think, once de-regulation occurs, the opportunities for and access to gambling will be like no other country really. I cannot think of another country . . . Take somewhere like Canada, which has also had a massive gambling explosion, they only have half our population in a country that geographically is so much bigger than ours. You cannot always compare like with like. Every country I have looked at that has de-regulated in a big way has seen an increase in problem gambling, and I do not see why that should not occur here, but there will be a different culture in terms of what people will enjoy gambling on. We only have 120 or so casinos in this country. If that number tripled, for instance, I would expect, because of that type of particular gambling, that it would attract a new clientele and there would be new problem gamblers as a result of that particular type of activity.

  Professor Orford: At the other end of the scale, Sweden is a country with what looks like very tight regulation on gambling and a more restricted range of types of gambling in Sweden. The Government is much more heavily involved in actually running gambling facilities and so on. So you have Australia at one end, I think, as Professor Griffiths has described, and Sweden at the other end and very tightly regulated.

  Q251  Jeff Ennis: May I ask one supplementary, Chairman, going back to the effects on adolescents and the attraction of machines, et cetera. Do we have any evidence now, because of the enormous expansion in home entertainment—computer games, X-boxes and that sort of thing, to which a lot of young lads in particular are drawn—that that sort of home entertainment facility is having any effect on the potential for problem gambling either one way or the other?

  Professor Griffiths: I have spent as many years studying things like video game addiction as I have slot-machine addiction. It is quite obvious that there is an overlap between the kind of people who get heavily involved in playing video games and heavily involved in playing slot-machines. Sue Fisher, again, has also done a lot of work on that in this country. The good news if you are addicted, for instance, to a home video game console is that the financial consequences are a lot less. If I am playing 12 hours a day on a home video console, the financial consequences are minimal compared with playing 12 hours a day on a slot-machine, although behaviourally it might be doing the same thing, basically, playing game after game, playing for long periods of time. I have always argued that the philosophy of both players is exactly the same: to stay on the machine for as long as possible, using the least amount of money. Fortunately, slot-machines are very expensive to play whereas video games are a lot cheaper. Even if you play in a video game arcade, you can make 20 pence or 50 pence last a long time if you are a very skilful player. The people who are supposedly skilful on slot-machines will not get anywhere near that in terms of time. There is, I think, a large cross-over between the types of people who become addicted to video games and those who become addicted to slot-machines.

  Jeff Ennis: Thank you, Chairman.

  Viscount Falkland: That was an interesting point you made about the distance to travel and the effect that has on this. In visiting France, I have noted in the past there has been a prohibition on having casinos in the Paris area, and only having them in holiday resorts, for example. In this country we are seeing it in terms of the spread of betting opportunities where we have areas of high density of population. Would you see that as a concern that needs to be addressed, for example, in Manchester, Glasgow, Liverpool, areas of high concentration like that, and that the local authorities should take cognisance of the fact that they should make it more difficult for people to go to casinos by extending the distance between them? It is a question, is it not, of trying to persuade people not to proliferate in particularly high density areas?

  Professor Griffiths: I think, again, it is a difficult issue. Could I just state for the record that I am not anti-gambling and I certainly do not want to stop adults doing what they want to do. If people want to go to casinos, that is absolutely fine by me. But, in all of this, whatever you do, there is always going to be a price to pay. If a mega-casino opens in Blackpool, my predictions would be that in the indigenous population of Blackpool you will see a rise in the number of problem gamblers who actually live in that locality, because they will have increased opportunity and access—basically because of a huge casino being open probably 24 hours a day. There is also the point that people will flock in, do an activity as part of a legitimate leisure activity, have a wonderful time in Blackpool, maybe come away having won, and that will be exported. It is not then going to Blackpool every night, but, having experienced what they have experienced there, it can be exported back to the towns and small places where they live if there is a gambling facility there. For the vast majority of people, it will be nothing but fun and exciting and a good time out, but, again, I do predict that you will see an increase in problem gambling within the local city or town or locality where those mega-casinos are located, plus for some people who have never even gambled before it will be exported back to where they live. That, again, will be to do with the availability hypothesis: it is because of increased access and opportunity.

  Dr Moran: I agree with my colleagues about this. I am not against gambling, I do it myself, but the point really is that, in view of the nature of gambling, when you participate in any type of activity it is vital that this should be a conscious decision which you have decided. In a free society, it is obviously highly appropriate that if people want to gamble they should be allowed to do so. What is tending to happen and will undoubtedly happen much more post-legislation is that people will stumble on it. It is very much a characteristic of internet gambling. If you look at the Times or the broadsheets on the internet, at the top there is a free bet: Why not have a go? This is the insidious aspect of the whole situation. But gambling by its very nature must be treated with respect. That does not mean to say that it should be banned or it should be avoided, but it must be treated with respect. Then you are far less likely to get into difficulties. Many of the proposals that are incorporated in the draft Gambling Bill, while on the one hand are saying we want "destination gambling"—and that is a term which is commonly used in the proposed legislation—in practice the situation will be one in which people will stumble on it. And that is the danger.

  Chairman: We will come back to that in a moment. Lord Walpole will come back to this issue of stimulation.

  Q252  Lord Walpole: Is the existing policy of unstimulated demand a necessary component of good gambling regulation, or can the Bill succeed in giving the industry more freedom to stimulate demand and also maintain low levels of problem gambling?

  Professor Orford: I am assuming that we are now into an era where unstimulated demand is a thing of the past. We are into some degree of stimulation in demand. The question, it seems to me is how do we get a balance whereby we do not increase the negative side, as you have said, too much. There are three areas I want to raise. One is something that I suppose would come under the heading of advertising standards. It does seem to me important that in society the gambling industry advertises its products fairly. One of the things I am worried about now, in the light of all that we have said here, is those "amusement with prizes". If the Government is determined that this anomaly should continue of child gambling, I think it should be called as such. I do not think people should be induced to play gaming machines by saying this is not gambling at all, these are actually amusements. I would claim that is actually contrary to advertising standards. Similarly, anything which claims that a form of gambling that is clearly not a matter of skill is actually a matter of skill, would, it seems to me, be against advertising standards. Anything that unfairly promotes gambling I would think should not be correct. The other thing is about undue inducements. I would have thought this is a matter of reputation of the gambling industry. We are told that the gambling industry has a good reputation in this country and has done for a number of years, and I think that is correct, but it does seem to me that if the gambling industry were to follow some of the things that we are told have happened in other countries it would lose that reputation. For example, I understand that in other countries free alcohol is provided in casinos. I would interpret that, and I am sure some other people would interpret that, as getting people intoxicated in order to more easily take money off them. I would have thought a gambling industry would rapidly get a bad reputation if it does that. I would have thought that was undue inducement. The other area to which I would call attention is to do with credit, because inherent in the habitual nature of gambling is the chasing of losses and a lot of people put that central to the idea of developing a problem with gambling. There is the idea of accumulating your winnings and not having your winnings paid to you before you bet again. Psychologically, it seems to me, it is bad and it is encouraging problem gambling to say to somebody, "Do you want your winnings? Or shall I keep them for you to bet again?" I think it is important that people get their winnings back each time, before they then make the choice to bet again. Personally, I think credit cards are dangerous. We live in a society now where credit card debt is a major national problem, so I would have thought allowing people to bet with credit cards was a bad thing. I do not think there should be cash machines anywhere near where you actually gamble: I think you should have to go a distance in order to get fresh cash. I think there should be methods whereby you are given feed-back about the amount you are gambling and you should be able to state a limit at the beginning of gambling and be told whether you are near to exceeding that limit or not. Those are just examples, but there is a whole set of things which I think comes under the area of credit and the way money is transacted in gambling, because all those things are central to the idea of how you develop habitual gambling.

  Q253  Lord Walpole: Do you think one-armed bandits should have health warnings on them—like a packet of cigarettes?

  Professor Griffiths: I certainly think people should be informed of what the pay-back rates are on the machine, and it would be a good idea to give them a running total of how much they have spent. I would say about slot-machines and gambling in general that we can all make conscious decisions beforehand about what we are doing to do and that this is our spending limit, but, as a person who plays slot-machines a lot in the name of research, I can tell you now that when you are actually playing on a slot-machine—and I sit here knowing all the probabilities, I know all about them—when I am in mid-action playing a slot-machine, all of that goes totally out of the window. You are totally in action, basically adrenaline is running round your body, and all those rational decisions you make are completely gone when you are actually in the gambling situation. A lot of problem gamblers will say that when they are in the gambling situation there are in a dissociated state: they do not actually feel they are the body they were. I have done it myself. I am not a problem gambler at all, but I know for a fact that I can feel out of myself and feel totally in escape mode when I am engaged in a particular form of gambling. That is the problem, even when you educate people—and I would like to think I am an educated person. It is like being under the influence of alcohol: when I have had a few drinks, I know what the effects are but my behaviour may be totally different and irrational when I am under the influence.

  Q254  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: Could I ask Professor Orford to add to his list of possible ways of breaking addictions on machines. In Australia, particularly in Victoria, smoking is now prohibited in the poker arcades for two reasons. One is obviously for public health reasons and the other is because it is seen as a way of interrupting the addiction. Would you see that as a useful measure to be adopted?

  Professor Orford: To ban smoking?

  Q255  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: At the machines.

  Professor Orford: The argument for that I have not quite understood.

  Q256  Lord Faulkner of Worcester: If you need another cigarette, you will stop.

  Professor Orford: It creates a break. It would not be my number one factor. I can see the logic of that, but it does not seem the most obvious thing to do.

  Professor Griffiths: I think you will find that people who really want to smoke more than they want to play will obviously stay out and smoke rather than play. We know for a fact that in Las Vegas people who drink a lot, for instance, will not drink a lot while gambling, because it will make them go to the toilet. They will either drink very short drinks so they do not have to go to the toilet so often, or if they get the urge they will urinate where they are on the seat. It is a common thing in Las Vegas casinos to find under the chairs big pools of urine from those people who are so into their gambling. My guess is that most people are not going to put themselves in that situation, so I agree with Jim, really, that the idea of the smoking link is not necessarily a good one.

  Dr Moran: I think more significant is the presence of alcohol. Alcohol impairs judgment. It clearly increases impulsivity. Therefore, the association between gambling and alcohol is, I think, a very hazardous one.

  Q257  Lord Mancroft: One of the features of the Government's policy which at least will limit negative social problems—and we have talked about this before but I would like to come back to it again—is that they believe destination gambling is better than casual gambling: the fact that gambling is in one place and people have to go there to do it will lessen the negative impacts. Do you think that is right? We have talked about conscious decisions and there would need to be a conscious decision to go to Blackpool or wherever it may be to do it. Is that a factor?

  Professor Griffiths: The problem is that a whole load of things are going on at once. If you are going to have destination resorts but also then increase the amount that people can gamble on the internet through interactive television or on their mobile phones, it makes the idea of destination gambling somewhat redundant anyway. My guess is that for 99 per cent of the people who go to a destination to gamble, like myself when I go to Las Vegas or wherever, it is because I think I am going to have a fun time. I do not go there to win money. If I win, that is a bonus. When I go to my local casino in Nottingham, I go there to have a meal, be with friends, have a talk or whatever and the gambling is incidental. My guess is that for most people who go to destination resorts that would be their aim, just to have a fun time out. Yes, they may win some, they may lose some, but the point is that this is not being done in isolation. Basically de-regulation is occurring in lots of different areas and we now have lots or remote forms of gambling that people can do. If I wanted to, I could probably link up with the internet and sit outside in this corridor and through my mobile phone gamble if I want to. It makes the idea of a destination resort somewhat redundant really. If this was the only thing that was happening, then you might see it as a good thing, but the point is that de-regulation is going on in lots of different spheres and not just in this one aspect.

  Q258  Chairman: This is an important point. One of the things you have just said—and forgive me if I have it wrong—is that if people go to a destination resort to enjoy the fun of gambling, that is why people would go. If you simply want to gamble, you do not need to go there at all.

  Professor Griffiths: Yes.

  Q259  Chairman: There is a very clear difference between the two forms.

  Professor Griffiths: I am sure there is for some people. But it was also my argument, as I was saying before, that you get those people who just want to go for the fun element, but when they get there and they are in gambling mode then irrationality can creep in as well. Thankfully, for most people it is not going to be something they are going to lose control of. I love going in big casinos around the world—I am sure most people do—I am not anti them at all, but where you introduce lots of big casinos everywhere you will increase the amount of gambling.

  Dr Moran: In the whole notion of destination gambling, I think in a very similar way throughout this whole business of this new legislation, there is an awful lot of confusion. The words "destination gambling" can be used in more than one way. If I decide to go to a betting shop, that is destination gambling. The Government at times uses it in that way and at other times it talks about destination gambling in terms of going to a future Blackpool, which is really going to be the English version of Las Vegas. These two are totally different ideas.


 
previous page contents next page

House of Lords home page Parliament home page House of Commons home page search page enquiries index

© Parliamentary copyright 2004
Prepared 7 April 2004